Shop By:

Beginning Literacy with Language


Beginning Literacy with Language

Young Children Learning at Home and School
Volume Editors: David K. Dickinson Ed.D., Patton O. Tabors Ed.D.

ISBN: 978-1-55766-479-2
Pages: 432
Copyright: 2001
Availability:
Available Stock
Request Print Exam Copy
Paperback $36.95 Qty:

Size:  6.0 x 9.0
Stock Number:  64792
Format:  Paperback
Promotion Code: 
In this fascinating book, you'll travel into the homes and schools of over 70 young children from diverse backgrounds and observe parent-child and teacher-child interactions. Through research gathered in the Home School Study of Language and Literacy Development, the authors share with you the relationship they've found between these critical, early interactions and children's kindergarten language and literacy skills.

You'll explore both the home and school environments of children at ages 3, 4, and 5 and see how families talk to their young children during everyday activities like book reading, toy play, and mealtimes. You'll also examine children's conversations throughout the classroom day and consider how teachers strive to support children's development. In each chapter, you'll

  • see how the data was collected
  • read actual transcripts of parent-child and teacher-child interactions
  • recognize how these interactions relate to later development
  • get suggestions for supporting children's language and literacy development
  • learn how these findings play out in the lives of four of the children in the study

Find out how young children's home and classroom experiences during the preschool years are related to their kindergarten language and literacy skills, and discover the kinds of conversations that make a difference.

Review and Rate this Item
Be the first to submit a customer review on this product!
Review and Rate this Item

Reviews

Review by: Courtney Cazden, Harvard Graduate School of Education

"Pay attention to the title! Beginning Literacy with Language is the bottom-line message of this longitudinal research on the language and literacy development of Head Start children all the way from preschool through 7th grade. Rich oral language opportunities matter a lot, at home and maybe even more at school. An encouraging and inspiring account of how schools, in early childhood and beyond, can make a difference."

Review by: Edward Zigler

"Vivid descriptions and thoughtful explanations of language development in young children. The book has great value to social scientists studying this enticing field. It also conveys a valuable message to parents and early childhood educators that their everyday interactions with small children form the abilities they will need to achieve literacy as they progress through formal schooling."

Review: Education Review

"Parents who are interested in day-to-day interaction opportunities will find the 'suggestions for parents' sections helpful, as they provide ideas without a lot of technical jargon and research methodologies. Educators will applaud this book for the in-depth research, references, statistics, charts, and actual classroom observations and transcripts. Professors in an academic Education Department may also wish to review a copy for use in an early childhood education curriculum."

Review: Harvard Educational Review

"An excellent resource for an academic course on child development, as it presents a broad picture of the applications of language development theory in education. It also has an instructive quality for the practitioner who may want a summary of theories regarding language and literacy development and empirical findings organized and presented by influential figures in this area of child development. Parents will also find this book useful. After each chapter, the editors present a series of recommendations of best practices for home and school environments."

Foreword
Sue Bredekamp

  1. Language Development in the Preschool Years
Catherine E. Snow, Patton O. Tabors, and David K. Dickinson
Importance of Language Development in the Preschool Years
Anatomy of a Research Study
The Portrait Children
Purpose of this Book

I: Supporting Language and Literacy Development in the Home
Patton O. Tabors
  1. Parents and Children Reading Books Together
Jeanne M. DeTemple
Importance of Book Reading in Children's Development of Language and Literacy Skills
Talk During Book Reading in the Home-School Study
Home Support for Literacy
Discussion
Suggestions for Parents
  1. Playing at Home: The Talk of Pretend Play
Jane R. Katz
Defining Social Pretend Play
Theoretical Views on the Development of Social Pretend Play
Analyzing the Play Sessions
Suggestions for Parents
  1. Eating and Reading: Links Between Family Conversations with Preschoolers and Later Language and Literacy
Diane E. Beals
Mealtime Data Collection
Literacy Outcomes
Conclusions
Suggestions for Parents
  1. You Know What Oxygen Is?: Learning New Words at Home
Patton O. Tabors, Diane E. Beals, and Zehava O. Weizman
Learning New Words
Support for Rare-Word Use in the Different Conversational Contexts
Relationships Between Rare Words and Kindergarten Vocabulary
Conclusions
Suggestions for Parents
  1. Home Language and Literacy Environment: Final Results
Patton O. Tabors, Kevin A. Roach, and Catherine E. Snow
Magnet Task and Science Process Talk
Home Language and Literacy Environment: Putting the Pieces Together
Home Language and Literacy Environment and Kindergarten Skills
Discussion
Conclusions
Section II: Supporting Language and Literacy Development in the Preschool Classroom
David K. Dickinson and Miriam W. Smith
Prior Studies of Preschool Supports for Language and Literacy
Summary
Classroom Data Collection
Overview of the Chapters in this Section
  1. Children's Experiences in Preschool
Miriam W. Smith
Methodological Considerations
Focus Teachers
Teachers' Pedagogical Beliefs
Teachers' Classroom Practices
Conclusions
  1. Book Reading in Preschool Classrooms: Is Recommended Practice Common?
David K. Dickinson
How Effective Book-Reading Practices Support Literacy Development
Mariana's Classroom: A Didactic Instruction Approach
Todd's Classroom: A Constructivist Approach
Place of Book Reading in the Classroom Day Across All Classrooms
How Two Teachers Read Books
Overall Style of Reading Books
Book-Reading Styles and Group Management
Content of Book Discussions Across Classrooms
Conclusions
Suggestions for Teachers
  1. Language Opportunities During Mealtimes in Preschool Classrooms
Linda R. Cote
Role of Mealtimes in the Head Start Day
Comparisons of the Quality of Mealtime Conversations
Long-Term Implications of Language Use During Mealtimes
Suggestions for Teachers
Conclusions
  1. Large-Group and Free-Play Times: Conversational Settings Supporting Language and Literacy Development
David K. Dickinson
Background Research
Profile Classrooms: Astra's and Casey's Classroom Days
Examining Large-Group Activities in Our Classrooms
Examining Free-Play Activities in Our Classrooms
Shift Between Preschool and Kindergarten
How We Examined Children's Conversations
Relationships Between Language Experiences and Development in Kindergarten
Contributions of Group Times to Language and Literacy in Kindergarten
Contributions of Free Play to Language and Literacy in Kindergarten
Conclusions
Suggestions for Teachers
  1. Putting the Pieces Together: Impact of Preschool on Children's Language and Literacy Development in Kindergarten
David K. Dickinson
Dimensions of Children's Classroom Experiences
Factors Affecting Teacher's Classroom Practices
Examining the Overall Impact of Preschool
Understanding How Classrooms Support Children's Development
Section III: Bringing Homes and Schools Together
David K. Dickinson and Patton O. Tabors
  1. Parent Involvement as a Link Between Home and School
Michelle V. Porche
Measuring Parent Involvement in the Home-School Study
Conclusions
  1. Homes and Schools Together: Supporting Language and Literacy Development
Patton O. Tabors, Catherine E. Snow, and David K. Dickinson
Long-Term Importance of Kindergarten Language and Literacy Skills
Linking Home and School Data
Implications for Practioners and Policymakers
Conclusions

Appendix: Home-School Study Data
References
Index

Language Development in the Preschool Years

Parents and early childhood educators want children to become good readers and writers. They are fully aware of how crucial reading and writing skills are to school success. But parents and early childhood educators may not know how important language development is in preparing preschool-age children for later literacy development. The purpose of this book is to provide information to parents and early childhood educators about the connections between young children's early language development and later literacy development so that they can support and facilitate young children's language skills both at home and in early care and education.

The material in this book is based on the findings of a research project, the Home-School Study of Language and Literacy Development, carried out since 1987 by a collaborative research team composed of members from the Harvard Graduate School of Education; Tufts University; Clark University; and the Education Development Center in Newton, Massachusetts. Researchers in this study have collected data in the homes and preschool, elementary school, and high school classrooms of a group of children from low-income families, starting when the children were 3 years old. This book reports on the information from the preschool and kindergarten period-a period that we have found makes crucial contributions in preparing children for their later literacy achievement.

In this book we not only present the findings from this research study but also make the findings come alive by presenting examples of the types of language that were audiotaped during visits to the homes and classrooms of these children, as well as quotes from teacher and mother interviews. Furthermore, to personalize the findings of the study, we have chosen four children and their families as examples of the overall group of children being studied. These children and their families are introduced later in this chapter, and further information about their lives-at home and in preschool-appears throughout the chapters that follow.

IMPORTANCE OF LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT IN THE PRESCHOOL YEARS

As mentioned, the focus of the Home-School Study is on young children's literacy development-how children become readers and writers. When we started this study, there was considerable interest in class differences with regard to children's literacy achievement. These socioeconomic differences led many researchers to focus on children's access to information about literacy as a determinant of their success in school. It was argued that before ever getting to school, some children have lots of opportunities at home to learn letters and sounds and to learn about handling books, making lists, writing notes, and other uses of literacy. These opportunities, which are generally available to children in more middle-class homes, might explain their literacy success.

Yet other researchers noted the robust relationship between reading achievement and vocabulary and suggested that individual and social class differences in children's vocabulary development would explain their reading outcomes. Although little work had been done on precisely how children developed larger vocabularies, it was clear that, in general, children from families with higher incomes and children of more highly educated mothers did have larger vocabularies at school entry than children from low-income families. This, too, might explain differences in reading outcomes.

Though we were not rejecting either of the previous explanations, we started this study with an interest in another aspect of language skill as well. We were particularly interested in children's experience with language that replicates some of the demands of literacy-that is, talk that requires participants to develop understandings beyond the here and now and that requires the use of several utterances or turns to build a linguistic structure, such as in explanations, narratives, or pretend. We call this type of talk extended discourse, and looking for differential opportunities to develop extended discourse skill was one of the goals of this project.

Why would anyone seek the sources of success at literacy in the domains of vocabulary or extended discourse skills? Ultimately, reading is a linguistic activity. Of course, there are specifically literate facts and procedures having to do with letters and with the sounds they represent that readers need to master. Increasingly, though, evidence suggests that learning about letters and sounds presupposes knowing a lot about the internal structure of words-knowledge that is hard to acquire without first knowing a lot of words. Furthermore, even the fairly straightforward system of how letters represent sounds is simplified if children can learn and practice it by reading words that they know. This is why beginning reading texts include only simple and highly frequent words.

All too soon, however, children, including many who have a reasonable grasp of letter-sound correspondences, encounter difficulties in reading if the texts they are reading in third or fourth grade use words they are not familiar with. Furthermore, such texts may deal with topics the children know little about, making comprehension a chore. And such texts are likely to include complex grammar and to use specific linguistic cues to indicate how the information presented is organized. If children have not learned these grammatical and organizational constructions orally, they will have difficulty using them while reading. We started this project, then, with the hypothesis that young children's language learning constitutes one important early step toward success in literacy.

In 1987, when we began the data collection process, the conviction that language was a crucial precursor to literacy was based more on intuition than on evidence. More recently, though, this conviction has come to be widely shared. Whereas we have long known that children's word knowledge is closely linked to reading accomplishments (Anderson & Freebody, 1981), more recent work has shown strong relationships between children's early language skills and later reading abilities (Hart & Risley, 1995; Purcell-Gates, 1988; Walker, Greenwood, Hart, & Carta, 1994). Other studies have shown that the levels of language and literacy skills that children have in kindergarten and first grade are strong predictors of achievement many years later (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997).

Understanding the role of language in fostering children's later literacy skills has important implications for all children, but it is of special significance for those who work with low-income families. Children growing up in these families are more likely to have difficulties with learning to read than children from middle-class families, and these gaps in performance begin to appear as early as kindergarten (Brizius & Foster, 1993; Dickinson & Snow, 1987). These early disadvantages can have serious long-term effects because children who experience reading difficulties in the middle grades are more likely than children without reading difficulties to drop out of school later in their academic careers (Barrington & Hendricks, 1989; Lloyd, 1978).

Differences in early achievement linked to parental income levels suggest that families vary in how effectively they support children's early language growth. One factor seems to be that children in families with limited incomes typically have fewer conversations with adults and are exposed to far fewer words than more advantaged children are. As a result, they tend to have smaller vocabularies than children from families with higher incomes (Hart & Risley, 1995). Also, these children tend to have fewer opportunities to hear books read aloud on a regular basis, thereby losing out on an experience known to support language growth (Zill, Collins, West, & Hausken, 1995). Though many studies have confirmed the existence of quantitative differences in the language experiences of children from low- and higher-income families, the Home-School Study presents an in-depth view of how parents and children in low-income families actually talk together in a variety of settings and the ways that this talk can support children's language growth.

The fact that children from low-income homes need additional support during the preschool years has long been recognized, as evidenced by the establishment of Head Start in the 1960s. Research conducted over two decades has given some evidence that Head Start and preschool programs similar to Head Start are benefiting low-income children (Barnett, 1995). Also, several major studies that have evaluated overall program quality have found that strong programs are more likely than programs of lesser quality to benefit preschool-age children (Bryant, Lau, Burchinal, & Sparling, 1994; Layzer, Goodson, & Moss, 1993; McCartney, Scarr, Phillips, & Grajek, 1985). Studies such as these look across many programs, identifying fairly general features of programs that are indicators of high quality, such as qualifications of staff and the adult-child ratio. Our research complements these studies by taking a more fine-grained look at the nature of what goes on in preschool classrooms. As a result, the findings of the Home-School Study can provide some concrete guidance regarding areas of preschool classroom life that are most likely to have beneficial effects on children's language and literacy development.

Educational Implications

The importance of early childhood experiences with rich language input and appropriate exposure to the uses and functions of print was emphasized by a series of national organizations in the late 1990s. The International Reading Association and the National Association for the Education of Young Children jointly issued a position statement in 1998 emphasizing the appropriateness of providing rich language and literacy experiences in preschool classrooms. A report from the National Academy of Sciences entitled Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998) argued that attention to children's language and literacy environment during the preschool years constitutes a crucial prevention effort and concluded, furthermore, that preschool classrooms serving young children at risk are often somewhat impoverished language and literacy environments. Federal legislators further endorsed this perspective when writing the Head Start Amendments of 1998 (PL 105-285) by setting specific goals for the knowledge about literacy and language that 4-year-olds in Head Start would be expected to achieve.

In other words, there seems to be a consensus that the environments of young children should be language-rich, with lots of words used during interesting conversations, and should be enriched by stories and explanations. The study presented in this book shows what such an environment looks like at home and at preschool, under which conditions it is likely to emerge, and precisely how it relates to children's learning.

In this book, we present descriptions of the language and literacy environments of 74 young children from low-income families. We relate our descriptions of the crucial features of those environments — at home and at preschool — to the children's accomplishments at the end of kindergarten. Of course, children at the end of kindergarten are typically not yet reading, so they can hardly yet have started to display reading difficulties, either. But we do know from a wide array of research carried out by others and from our own ongoing studies of the children in the Home-School Study that some aspects of children's skills at the end of kindergarten predict later literacy accomplishments quite well. Thus, even though the topic of this book is limited to the very beginnings of reading development, we have a basis for saying that the features of homes and preschool classrooms that support children's literacy in kindergarten help to pave the way for children's later reading success.

Although the analyses in this book are focused on the preschool to kindergarten period of the Home-School Study, the research team has continued to visit the homes and classrooms of the children in the study. Home visits were made to the families when the children were 7, 9, and 12 years old, and school visits have been made each year up to sophomore year in high school except for fifth and eighth grade. During these visits we have continued to collect data on language interactions at home, on family support for literacy skills, on types of reading instruction in classrooms, and on the children's accomplishments in the domains of language and literacy. The final chapter provides some updates concerning the participants of the study when they were in fourth and seventh grade.

So, why does the information in this book stop at kindergarten? We have found that knowing how well these children did on language and literacy tasks as kindergartners tells us a lot about how they went on to succeed or to struggle in their later schooling. Furthermore, we have found that the foundation of the children's kindergarten abilities can be found in their home and preschool classroom language and literacy environments. What the Home-School Study tells us is that the preschool years are critical.

Excerpted from chapter 1 of Beginning Literacy with Language: Young Children Learning at Home and at School, edited by David K. Dickinson, Ed.D., & Patton O. Tabors, Ed.D.

Copyright © 2001 by Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. All rights reserved. No part of this excerpt may be reproduced or reprinted without permission in writing from the publisher.

Your Cart

(0 product)
Empty
Customer Service
  • Privacy Policy
  • Permissions
  • Catalogs
  • Copyright